Running Current Through Your Brain Improves Performance, Not As Likely To Kill You As You Think

For many people placing a nine-volt battery on their tongue, like sex, can be a fun, exciting activity, sometimes resulting in death. Based on this observation, Reinhart and Woodman (2014) decided to turn the brain into an improvised battery by placing electrodes on different areas of the skull and zapping it with enough current to toast a frozen hotpocket. If this sounds insane to you, then you are obviously not a cognitive neuroscientist - as I have said before, we live for these kinds of experiments.

However, the researchers had good reasons for doing this. First of all, they are scientists, and they did not spend nine years on their doctorate only to justify themselves to the likes of you. Second, directly messing with brain activity can lead to valuable scientific insights, such as how much you have to pay an undergraduate to have them consent to turn their brain into a microwave oven. But third, and most important, delivering direct current through a pair of electrodes can increase or decrease certain patterns of neural activity - specifically, the error-related negativity (ERN) following an error trial.

The ERN is a negative deflection in voltage over the medial frontal lobes that correlates with behavior adjustment and error correction in the future. For example, if I commit what some narrow-minded, parochial individuals consider an error, such as asking out my girlfriend's sister, the larger my ERN is, the less likely I am to make that same mistake in the future. Similarly, with experiments such as the Stroop task, or any performance task, the larger the ERN after committing an error, the greater the probability of making a correct response on the next trial. Furthermore, whereas the ERN usually occurs immediately after the response is made, another related signal, the feedback related negativity (FRN) occurs once feedback is received. In sum, larger ERNs and FRNs generally lead to better future performance.

This is exactly what the experimenters manipulated when they sent current through the medial frontal area of the brain, corresponding to the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and supplementary motor area (SMA) cortical regions. The electrode over this area was changed to either a cathode (i.e., positively charged, or where the electrons flowed toward) or an anode (i.e., negatively charged, or where the electrons flowed away from). If the electrode was a cathode, the ERN decreased significantly, whereas if the electrode was an anode, the ERN significantly increased.

Figure 1 from Reinhart & Woodman (2014). Panels A and D represent the current distribution throughout the medial prefrontal cortex. B: Stop-signal task used in the experiment. A stop signal leads to a greater chance of screwing up, and the longer the delay between the cue and the stop signal, the more difficult it is to stop a response. This is what physicists and individuals with severe incontinence refer to as an "event horizon." C: Placement of Cathode or Anode on the medial frontal surface, along with a sham condition. Lower panel: Difference in ERN and FRN dependent on whether the fronto-medial electrode is an Anode or Cathode.

As interesting as these neural differences are, however, the real punch of the paper lies in the behavioral changes. Participants who had an anode placed over their cingulate and SMA areas not only showed greater ERN and FRN profiles, but also steep gains in their accuracy and improvements in reaction time. For regular trials which did not include a distracting stop signal, anode subjects were markedly faster than in the cathode and sham conditions, and in both regular and stop-signal trials, accuracy nearly reached a hundred percent.


Nor were these gains limited to the duration of the experiment; in fact, behavioral improvements could last as long as five hours after switching on the current. These results make for wild and reckless speculations about what could be done with this kind of setup; one could imagine creating caps for students which get them "juiced up" for exams, hats for the elderly to help them find their Mysteriously Disappearing Reading Glasses, or modified helmets for soldiers which allow them get even better at BSU (blowing stuff up). Because, after all, what's the use of a scientific result if you can't weaponize it?

More figures and results from experiments further extending and confirming their results can be seen in the paper, found here.

The Will to Persevere, Induced by Deep Brain Stimulation

Scientists, and neuroscientists in particular, are an odd bunch, with complex, multifaceted personalities. on the one hand they can be reclusive, socially awkward, and pretentious; but, in their defense, they make an honest effort to try and make up for these defects by being completely nuts.

For example, although most neuroscientists won't admit it, deep down, in their heart of hearts, somewhere in the left atrium, each and every one of feels a little twinge of excitement at the prospect of sticking electrodes somewhere in a person's brain and delivering electrical shocks. Seriously. Just ask any self-described neuroscience researcher what he would love to do most, and nine times out of ten he will say "Stick things inside someone's brain and inject enough electricity into it to light up a small amusement park." Only rarely will he give a more reasoned, more mature answer, such as "Purchase a motorcycle," or "Become an adult film star."

In any case, neuroscientists are usually prevented from acting out their sick fantasies by institutional review boards, or IRBs, which, from a neuroscientist's point of view, exist solely to be squeamish buzzkills and to put your experiments under review for a length of time equivalent to the gestation period of a yak. However, every once in a while there will be a case where an epileptic patient is undergoing a craniotomy, for example, or where a fellow neuroscientist is receiving extensive brain surgery after his latest motorcycle accident; and these cases, in addition to being like the Irish Sweepstakes for neuroscientists, can also yield valuable insights about how direct stimulation of cortical and subcortical areas can induce different physiological and cognitive states.

A recent example of this type of research appeared in a paper in the journal Neuron a couple of months ago, by Parvizi et al. Two epilepsy patients had deep-brain electrodes implanted in their brain, and the researchers were particularly interested in those electrodes located within the midcingulate region of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). After delivering small bursts of electricity to these electrodes, the patients reported higher levels of autonomic system activity, including increased heart rate and alertness, along with a feeling of foreboding but a concomitant feeling of resolve to overcome the intangible "challenge" that they felt. A follow-up resting-state analysis showed that both of these seed stimulation regions were hubs of a widespread cingulo-opercular network, similar to the typical coactivation of cingulate and insula responses observed in most studies examining the medial prefrontal cortex, and also involved in detecting emotional salience and sustaining goal-directed activity.

Figure 1 from Parvizi et al, showing the stimulation site in the midcingulate region for both patients, as well as more remote stimulation sites for comparison.


Figure 2 from Parvizi et al depicting a resting-state functional connectivity analysis using the midcingulate as a seed region.

The authors labeled these feelings of wanting to overcome a formidable challenge as the "will to persevere," a phrase I think will be variously interpreted, but which seems apt enough for the current paper. However, one concern that popped into my head while reading through the article (dons reviewer glasses, purses lips disapprovingly) was: Is it really a will to persevere, or just a general increase in autonomic nervous system arousal (i.e., the sympathetic branch)? The "will to persevere" reported here may be the patient's interpretation of his increased heart rate, which, given the circumstances of the experiment and the patient's undergoing surgery to treat his epilepsy, may reflect his desire for a successful outcome of the surgery. Placing the patient in a different environment or with different circumstances - say, locking him in a room with one of the facehuggers from the movie Alien - may lead to a reinterpretation of the same increased arousal as fear, instead of a general willingness to overcome the challenge that lays in front of him.

In any case, these results, coupled with the lack of emotional response to electrical charges delivered to control stimulation sites and sham stimulations, lends support to the theory that the midcingulate region plays some kind of role in motivation, and that stimluation to this region may have practical applications for disorders involving pathologically low amounts of motivation, such as major depression and senioritis; disorders which, I might add, I am fully qualified to treat with open-brain surgery and a homemade electrical stimulation kit consisting of copper wire and a couple of lemons. Just give me a call.


Link to paper (including video of interview with subject 1; scroll to bottom of page): http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0896627313010301